Animal Flatmates: Deep into their minds

Nonfiction, Science & Nature, Nature, Animals, Science
Cover of the book Animal Flatmates: Deep into their minds by Rachele Malavasi, Rachele Malavasi
View on Amazon View on AbeBooks View on Kobo View on B.Depository View on eBay View on Walmart
Author: Rachele Malavasi ISBN: 9781370768301
Publisher: Rachele Malavasi Publication: August 5, 2016
Imprint: Smashwords Edition Language: English
Author: Rachele Malavasi
ISBN: 9781370768301
Publisher: Rachele Malavasi
Publication: August 5, 2016
Imprint: Smashwords Edition
Language: English

In the last forty years, the interest towards the mental skills of non-human animals (from now on, animals), their cognitive abilities and, more broadly, their behavior, has grown. Animals are able to form mental representations of reality, are able to recognize each other individually, to learn socially and to communicate intentionally. They have been declared “sentient beings” in the Treaty of Amsterdam, and thus able to feel good and suffer. What does it mean practically?
To “feel good” includes being able to express species-specific skills. Horses can travel up to 30km/day, and the functionality of their digestive system works at best in motion; they can recognize conspecifics and live in complex fission-fusion societies. Even so, many owners keep horses in “boxes” where their movements are deeply limited and their social life is reduced to a minimum (Chapter Two, by Pia Lucidi). Similarly, iguanas, present in domestic contexts as exotic pets, have a social life that includes peculiar forms of greeting, such as body and chin rubbing, tail wagging, head bobs, mutual tongue licking, and allogrooming (Chapter Five, by Gionata Stancher). Keeping iguanas in isolation excludes the possibility for them to perform these displays and engage in affiliative interactions with peers.
Other animals scarcely considered for their intellectual skills, yet often present on our plates, are chickens. To “avoid pain” also means to “have access to cures.” When given the opportunity, chickens prefer to feed on food integrated with painkillers: this act of self-medication suggests that chickens do perceive their pain and act to alleviate it (Chapter One, by Cinzia Chiandetti).
Even fish, for which the ability to perceive pain is still debated, should have the right to “feel good” in terms of “feeling safe”: some fish species engage in cooperative behaviors where they swim together toward a potential predator to inspect its degree of dangerousness (Chapter Four, by Maria Elena Miletto Petrazzini). Safety is in numbers!
In our city, block, building, or even in our apartment live many other visible (and invisible) species with incredible cognitive skills, single individuals that are motivated by the need to feel good and avoid pain as well. To “avoid pain” may be for example translated as “to avoid thirst or hunger.” So, fruit flies travel over 15 kilometers to find a water spot, and from taking-off to navigation and landing, sophisticate sensory-motor and cognitive abilities are needed (Chapter Seven, by Elisabetta Versace).
Of course, this book couldn’t miss a chapter on human’s best friend, dogs, so plain in expressing their feelings to the point of establishing an attachment bond with the owner (Chapter Six, by Tiziano Travain and Paola Maria Valsecchi). Similarly, cats lived in our houses for millennia and even evolved the ability to elaborate their vocalization by including the vocal elements of baby’s cries that solicit human females to provide the desired treats (Chapter Three, by Isabella Merola and Rachele Malavasi).
The aim of this volume is to promote, through knowledge, the welfare of some of the species that live closest to humans. We provide here an overview of the cognitive skills and their ontogeny of dogs, cats, fish, chickens, horses, fruit flies, and reptiles, without demanding completeness but rather focusing on some peculiar skills or aspects of each one. We choose to use a simple and accessible language: this volume may not only be of interest to academics but also to the broad public and associations for animal rights or safeguarding animal welfare. They would find in this volume an objective and accurate guide towards a deeper understanding of these species. The book has seven chapters, each written by a senior researcher and correlated with an in-depth information box. All contributions included in this volume come from Italian researchers in support of the cultural excellence of this country.

View on Amazon View on AbeBooks View on Kobo View on B.Depository View on eBay View on Walmart

In the last forty years, the interest towards the mental skills of non-human animals (from now on, animals), their cognitive abilities and, more broadly, their behavior, has grown. Animals are able to form mental representations of reality, are able to recognize each other individually, to learn socially and to communicate intentionally. They have been declared “sentient beings” in the Treaty of Amsterdam, and thus able to feel good and suffer. What does it mean practically?
To “feel good” includes being able to express species-specific skills. Horses can travel up to 30km/day, and the functionality of their digestive system works at best in motion; they can recognize conspecifics and live in complex fission-fusion societies. Even so, many owners keep horses in “boxes” where their movements are deeply limited and their social life is reduced to a minimum (Chapter Two, by Pia Lucidi). Similarly, iguanas, present in domestic contexts as exotic pets, have a social life that includes peculiar forms of greeting, such as body and chin rubbing, tail wagging, head bobs, mutual tongue licking, and allogrooming (Chapter Five, by Gionata Stancher). Keeping iguanas in isolation excludes the possibility for them to perform these displays and engage in affiliative interactions with peers.
Other animals scarcely considered for their intellectual skills, yet often present on our plates, are chickens. To “avoid pain” also means to “have access to cures.” When given the opportunity, chickens prefer to feed on food integrated with painkillers: this act of self-medication suggests that chickens do perceive their pain and act to alleviate it (Chapter One, by Cinzia Chiandetti).
Even fish, for which the ability to perceive pain is still debated, should have the right to “feel good” in terms of “feeling safe”: some fish species engage in cooperative behaviors where they swim together toward a potential predator to inspect its degree of dangerousness (Chapter Four, by Maria Elena Miletto Petrazzini). Safety is in numbers!
In our city, block, building, or even in our apartment live many other visible (and invisible) species with incredible cognitive skills, single individuals that are motivated by the need to feel good and avoid pain as well. To “avoid pain” may be for example translated as “to avoid thirst or hunger.” So, fruit flies travel over 15 kilometers to find a water spot, and from taking-off to navigation and landing, sophisticate sensory-motor and cognitive abilities are needed (Chapter Seven, by Elisabetta Versace).
Of course, this book couldn’t miss a chapter on human’s best friend, dogs, so plain in expressing their feelings to the point of establishing an attachment bond with the owner (Chapter Six, by Tiziano Travain and Paola Maria Valsecchi). Similarly, cats lived in our houses for millennia and even evolved the ability to elaborate their vocalization by including the vocal elements of baby’s cries that solicit human females to provide the desired treats (Chapter Three, by Isabella Merola and Rachele Malavasi).
The aim of this volume is to promote, through knowledge, the welfare of some of the species that live closest to humans. We provide here an overview of the cognitive skills and their ontogeny of dogs, cats, fish, chickens, horses, fruit flies, and reptiles, without demanding completeness but rather focusing on some peculiar skills or aspects of each one. We choose to use a simple and accessible language: this volume may not only be of interest to academics but also to the broad public and associations for animal rights or safeguarding animal welfare. They would find in this volume an objective and accurate guide towards a deeper understanding of these species. The book has seven chapters, each written by a senior researcher and correlated with an in-depth information box. All contributions included in this volume come from Italian researchers in support of the cultural excellence of this country.

More books from Science

Cover of the book Chernobyl's Vampires and Werewolves by Rachele Malavasi
Cover of the book Jacquelyn and the Sparkly Emo Vampire Goat by Rachele Malavasi
Cover of the book The Two Worlds by Rachele Malavasi
Cover of the book Arkon 8: Die Stunde des Smilers by Rachele Malavasi
Cover of the book Die Kugel von Kandra (Oliver Blue und die Schule für Seher − Buch Zwei) by Rachele Malavasi
Cover of the book Trade in the Ancient Sahara and Beyond by Rachele Malavasi
Cover of the book Uncle Petros and Goldbach's Conjecture by Rachele Malavasi
Cover of the book A Joosr Guide to... The Magic of Reality by Richard Dawkins: How We Know What’s Really True by Rachele Malavasi
Cover of the book God Saved Me from Death Row by Rachele Malavasi
Cover of the book Cronistoria di un omicidio by Rachele Malavasi
Cover of the book Law and Legalization in Transnational Relations by Rachele Malavasi
Cover of the book Cracked Open by Rachele Malavasi
Cover of the book Poverty Dictatorship Injustice by Rachele Malavasi
Cover of the book Mars and its Inhabitants by Rachele Malavasi
Cover of the book The Story of Light by Rachele Malavasi
We use our own "cookies" and third party cookies to improve services and to see statistical information. By using this website, you agree to our Privacy Policy